Psychology Child Psychology And Inherent Term Paper

PAGES
10
WORDS
2951
Cite

It is clear from the studies thus far examined (plus a few more) that the ability to distinguish between animate and inanimate objects develops much earlier than Piaget imagined. Second, while it is unclear from this study if the rules of grammar inform the child's sense of animacy or vice versa, we find that children significantly tend to attribute animate characteristics to a sentence's first noun and inanimate qualities to the second. In either case, clearly children are able to make early deductions about the characteristics of animate objects vs. inanimate ones. Perhaps they were able to draw conclusions about the most logical configuration about the sentence based on their preexisting knowledge of animacy, or perhaps the opposite is true. Perhaps the rules of grammar that have already been taught informed children's sense of animacy. Whichever the reality, the development of an understanding of the differences between animate and inanimate objects clearly occurs at a very young age. Other studies on the subject only confirm this position with other pieces of the overall puzzle of development. Another linguistic examination found that a child's ability to determine whether or not a sentence was anomalous is based on preexisting notions about animacy (Schwartz, 1980). In other words, a child's ability to tell if a sentence was constructed anomalously based on animacy characteristics was based on presupposition the child had regarding animacy. A young child, thus, who had not yet figured out that rocks don't move on their own might judge this sentence -- "The rock thought long and hard before deciding to roll down the hill" -- to be perfectly all right. An adult, on the other hand, would quickly point out the anomalous nature of this sentence because of the adult's more mature sense of animate vs. inanimate knowledge.

Schwartz (1980) found that a child's ability to judge a sentence anomalous was based on the preexisting knowledge of animacy. Unfortunately, since this study was designed to examine grammar development, its applicability to the question at hand is only tangential. It does however suggest that the development of knowledge of animate and inanimate differences precedes grammatical development, a finding that shapes our understanding of the previous study's findings (Dewart, 1979). That study found that children as young as five were able to determine animacy based on sentence structure, but was unclear about whether or not a knowledge of animacy preceded a knowledge of grammar. Schwartz (1980) seems to suggest that knowledge of animacy must come first. That would mean that development of knowledge of the difference between animate and inanimate objects must occur as young as age three or four, much younger than previously thought.

Of course, there are other means to test the development of this ability in children that cover topics other than grammar and linguistics. Greif and his fellow researchers (2006) recently tested thirty-two preschool age children, encouraging them to ask questions about the characteristics of images of animals and artifacts that were shown to them. The research methodology was simple: show children images of either animals or objects and then draw conclusions about their development based on the kind of questions the children employed to find out more about the images. Importantly, the questions that the children framed were never categorically inappropriate: no child asked what kind of babies a volcano had, or what was the favorite lunch of a street sign.

Instead, the children were more likely to ask questions that probed the function of the artifacts and the biological natures of the animals. The inevitably conclusion that the researchers came to was that the children at that age were already classifying artifacts based on their intended function, while they categorized animals by biological characteristics (Greif et al., 2006). This means, of course, that by the time that the children were investigated by the researchers, they already had a basic knowledge of the difference between animate and inanimate objects. If they did not already have in their minds a basic picture of what constituted the differences between these two, then they would have found it impossible -- or at least difficult -- to ask appropriately framed questions about the images the researchers showed them. The fact that the children had no difficulty in doing so means that they had already developed knowledge of the difference between animate and inanimate objects.

In fact, the most consistent aspect of these studies is that they all indicate that knowledge of animacy emerges much earlier than Piaget believed that it did. Tunmer (1985), in an examination of social vs. nonsocial cognition, concluded that children between the ages of four and seven...

...

Social cognition involves the actions of sentient, thinking beings. It is, in effect, psychology. Nonsocial cognition, on the other hand, is a product of physical events -- the realm of inanimate matter. While Tunmer argued that the ability to differentiate between social and nonsocial cognition occurred later than the ability to draw a line between animate and inanimate objects, his subjects still conform to the early development of this capacity.
Massey and Gelman (1988) also tested whether or not preschool-age children were capable of understanding the difference between animate and inanimate objects based on the ability of the former to self-initiate actions. The findings demonstrate that four-year-old were reliably accurate about which type of object could self-initiate action in a number of categories. Three-year-olds scored significantly above chance in all but one of the categories. This is an important final study to consider on the question of the development of animate and inanimate knowledge. Clearly, this study illustrates that this kind of knowledge is not innate or inborn, somehow coded into indivdual genes. If that were the case, then there shouldn't have been as significant a difference in the ability of the children to classifying objects based on the objects' ability to self-initiate action. If the aforementioned knowledge was built-in, it should be as robust in three-year-olds as it is in four-year-olds. In fact, we should see no discernable development of the ability to distinguish between animate and inanimate objects during childhood development.

This position is, of course, incorrect. Clearly, Massey and Gelman (1988) show that there is a quantifiable improvement in animacy conceptions as children age. The other studies presented above all conclude that this knowledge develops at a remarkably young age; however, none suggests that two-year-olds or infants have the ability to make this kind of distinction. The ability to do so is informed by environmental inputs and training -- both social and nonsocial training. It is not of issue that these children do not possess a genetic understanding of these concepts; what is incredibly intriguing and important is that not only is the process developmental but that it occurs at a fantastically rapid pace during early childhood.

Clearly, Piaget's early conclusions that the development of conceptions of animate and inanimate distinctions only develops by age eleven or twelve are entirely incorrect. Whatever the failings of Piaget's original studies, it is incredibly evident that the development of this type of knowledge about the world occurs at a significantly earlier phase of development than previously believed. The body of scholarly consensus is clear: by age five children have developed a clear understanding of the nature of animate and inanimate objects. Some studies point to the possibility that the development can occur earlier. There is no significant indication that children are plagued by a pervasive misapplication of animate characteristics onto inanimate objects until they reach adolescence. Whatever Piaget's contributions to developmental studies, his conclusions on animacy must be abandoned in favor of the weight of academic discourse on the subject.

Sources Used in Documents:

References

Dewart, M.H. (1979). Children's hypotheses about the animacy of actor and object nouns. British Journal of Psychology, 70(4), pp. 525-530.

Dolgin, K., & Behrend, D. (1984). Children's knowledge about animates and inanimates. Child Development, 55(4), pp. 1646-1650.

Greif, M.L., Nelson, D.G.K., Keil, F.C., and Gutierrez, F. (2006). What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Psychological Science, 17(6), pp. 455-459.

Inagaki, K. & Hatano, G. (2006). Young children's conception of the biological world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(4), pp. 177-181.


Cite this Document:

"Psychology Child Psychology And Inherent" (2007, December 05) Retrieved April 24, 2024, from
https://www.paperdue.com/essay/psychology-child-psychology-and-inherent-73487

"Psychology Child Psychology And Inherent" 05 December 2007. Web.24 April. 2024. <
https://www.paperdue.com/essay/psychology-child-psychology-and-inherent-73487>

"Psychology Child Psychology And Inherent", 05 December 2007, Accessed.24 April. 2024,
https://www.paperdue.com/essay/psychology-child-psychology-and-inherent-73487

Related Documents

These may include the parental workplace, school boards, social service agencies, and planning commissions." (Strengthening the Family: Implications for International Development, nd) Four: The Macro-system Macro-systems are 'blueprints' for interlocking social forces at the macro-level and their interrelationships in shaping human development. They provide the broad ideological and organizational patterns within which the meso- and exo-systems reflect the ecology of human development. Macro-systems are not static, but might change through evolution

Figure 1 portrays three of the scenes 20/20 presented March 15, 2010. Figure 1: Heather, Rachel, and Unnamed Girl in 20/20 Program (adapted from Stossel, 2010). Statement of the Problem For any individual, the death of a family member, friend, parent or sibling may often be overwhelming. For adolescents, the death of person close to them may prove much more traumatic as it can disrupt adolescent development. Diana Mahoney (2008), with the

Child Counseling
PAGES 5 WORDS 2053

Child Counseling The objective of this work is to provide viable research techniques to use in order to help a child and her family. This report represents a summary of Alicia Thomas, a nine-year-old African-American 4th grader with a series of legitimate medical as well as possibly psychosomatic physiological and psychological concerns. The young lady has been specifically diagnosed as having a duodenal ulcer with the inherent gastrointestinal symptoms including vomiting

Children in Sports; From a
PAGES 4 WORDS 1584

According to Smoll and Smith, there are two basic attitudes toward competition; an ego attitude and a mastery attitude. Parents who have an ego attitude toward their own competition -that is, they compete to win and to be better than others - are especially likely to be competitive with other parents about their child's achievements. Essentially, the parent goes from being proud to being boastful. These, then, are the four

The first stage of language acquisition is talking one word at a time. The child uses that single word to make requests and direct activities. The individual at this stage can be considered an "emergent communicator." Their use of language for the purpose of communication is in its beginning period. Thus, the articulations consists mainly of a single word, this word may capture an entire sentence. In stage, two the individual

The model of the "social structural child" sees the childhood as a social system comparable to the other social categories. Though, the childhood system is different from the others and even marginalized, fact well pointed out in the "minority group child" model. The model of the "tribal child" is more concentrated on the children's world, which is considered to have its own separate culture. The "socially constructed child" model