Sociocognitive Metaphors Constraints On Sociocognitive Essay

Infants that are securely attached, then, expect their figures of attachment to be readily available and are quickly and easily comforted if upset. Conversely, those infants that are not securely attached do not share this level of expectation. Among adults, secure attachments provide a base for caregiving and compassion (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2005). Conclusion

What then causes individuals to describe his or her reality in terms of noncommensurate physical qualities like cleanliness, verticality, weight or temperature? Landau et al. (2010) have provided a convincing argument that these kinds of sociocognitive metaphors are reflective of general basic processes that allow individuals to make the world make sense. However, when looking from the contextual framework of grounded cognition, the psychological importance of sociocognitive metaphors exceeds mental representation and even language. There are some sociocognitive metaphors that seem to provide greater universality that finds its foundation in bodily constraints and schemas that are relational, rooted in historic brain structures. While other sociocognitive metaphors are different across cultures but somehow emerge from very specific "cultural differences in embodiment" (IJzerman & Koole, 2011).

Thus, grounding sociocognitive metaphors may be increasingly assistive in elucidating motivational significance. The majority of the most frequently used sociocognitive metaphors that people are passionate and care deeply about such as morality, self, power and love. From a grounded cognition perspective, this is not coincidental. Sociocognitive metaphors, therefore, do not exist simply for the sake of mental representation alone. They exist for action as well. What makes metaphors meaningful may be directly correlated and linked to what motivates an individual. Being psychological and physically close to other individuals may be especially important in times when individuals think about the self as powerful and may be especially important as individuals prepare to use physical force (Schubert & Koole, 2009). As such, a grounded cognition perspective may offer a plausible explanation for the enduring psychological appeal of sociocognitive metaphors (IJzerman & Koole, 2011).

References

Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Barsalou, L. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-609.

Cohen, D., Leung, A., & IJzerman, H. (2009). Culture, psyche, and body make each other up. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1298-1299.

Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991). Social cognition. New York, NY: Random House.

Fiske, A. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of human relations.

New York: Free Press.

Fiske, A. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689-823.

Fiske, A. (2004). Relational models theory 2.0. In N. Hamslam (ed.), Relational models theory: A contemporary...

...

3-25), London, United Kingdom: Erlbaum.
Higgins, E. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability and salience. In E.T. Higgins & A.W. Kruglanski (eds), Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133-168). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Hove, M., & Risen, J. (2009). It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949-960.

IJzerman, H., & Koole, S. (2011). From perceptual rags to metaphoric riches -- Bodily, social and cultural constraints on Sociocognitive metaphors: comment on Landau, Meier, and Keefer. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 355-361.

IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. (2010). Temperature as a ground for social proximity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 867-873.

Johnson, C. (1997). Learnability in the acquisition of multiple senses: Sources reconsidered. In J. Moxley, J. Juge, & M. Juge (eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 469-480). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: HarperCollins.

Macrae, C., & Bodenhausen, G. (2000). Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93-120.

Macrae, C., Bodenhausen, G., Milne, A., & Jetten, J. (1994). Out of mind but back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808-817.

Mandler, J. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thoughts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. (2005). Attachment security, compassion, and altruism.

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 34-38.

Niedenthal, P., Barsalou, L., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184-211.

Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Schubert, T., & Koole, S. (2009). The embodied self: Making a fist enhances men's power-related self-conceptions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 828-834.

Smith, E. (1998). Mental representations and memory. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, 4th ed., pp. 391-445). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.

Smith, E., & Semin, G. (2004). Socially situated cognition: Cognition in its social context. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 53-117.

Stapel, D., & Koomen, W. (2000). How far do we go beyond information given? The impact of knowledge activation on interpretation and inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 19-37.

Williams, L., & Bargh, J. (2008). Keeping one's distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19, 302-308.

Williams,…

Sources Used in Documents:

References

Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Barsalou, L. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-609.

Cohen, D., Leung, A., & IJzerman, H. (2009). Culture, psyche, and body make each other up. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1298-1299.

Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (1991). Social cognition. New York, NY: Random House.


Cite this Document:

"Sociocognitive Metaphors Constraints On Sociocognitive" (2011, November 25) Retrieved April 20, 2024, from
https://www.paperdue.com/essay/sociocognitive-metaphors-constraints-on-47867

"Sociocognitive Metaphors Constraints On Sociocognitive" 25 November 2011. Web.20 April. 2024. <
https://www.paperdue.com/essay/sociocognitive-metaphors-constraints-on-47867>

"Sociocognitive Metaphors Constraints On Sociocognitive", 25 November 2011, Accessed.20 April. 2024,
https://www.paperdue.com/essay/sociocognitive-metaphors-constraints-on-47867

Related Documents

Metaphors, Similes, Analogies For most of us, a day without metaphors, similes and analogies is like a day without sunshine. For some of us, certain events or observations leave us at a loss for good descriptive words, and it is in those moments that a well-worn metaphor, simile or analogy is put to use. Specifically, a metaphor is a figure of speech in which an object or idea is used in

And yes, an occupational therapist may teach a patient how to hold a glass of milk and savor that nourishment, as I have savored what I have learned, which is simple yet essential. Important tasks are often taken for granted, like simple, nourishing milk. These simple physical, psychological, and social components of a healthy life are so vital to all human beings, regardless of their personal limitations and challenges. The

The danger that surrounds Huck and his friends in the book is also exciting, and lends much to the story in many places. It is Huck's first foray into the real world, and through the metaphor of the river, he and his friends get to share some very eye-opening experiences As the boys travel south, the river becomes more and more dangerous and hostile. Huck and Jim have to endure

metaphors in communication allows us to "tap into a richer vein of language and thought." (Kirby et al., 1999, p. 82) They allow us to better describe our world to those around us, than mere description alone, promoting more effective communication. By blending aspects of other ideas and images with the thoughts we are trying to convey, metaphors allow us to bring our ideas to life, and help others

metaphor has been considered a figure of speech generally used in literary creations in order to make the reader see some of the writer's ideas and visions. However, we could extrapolate this concept to a much higher level and be convinced that the metaphor is also a way of life and a way by which we form associations between our actions and other events in life. One such example would

metaphor use by using life compared to sports as its basis. The writer explains why life is more like boxing than it is running track to explore the many metaphoric opportunities the two sports provide regarding human life. LIFE IS MORE LIKE BOXING THAN RUNNING TRACK Using metaphoric examples is something that has been done in literature for many years. Metaphoric examples allow the writer to explain situations in terms that